Tuesday, July 16, 2013

Early Spatial Reasoning Predicts Later Creativity and Innovation, Especially in STEM Fields


Exceptional spatial ability at age 13 predicts creative and scholarly achievements more than 30 years later, according to results from a Vanderbilt University longitudinal study, published today in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

The study, conducted by David Lubinski and colleagues at Vanderbilt University Peabody College of education and human development, provides evidence that early spatial ability -- the skill required to mentally manipulate 2D and 3D objects -- predicts the development of new knowledge, and especially innovation in science, technology, engineering and mathematics (STEM) domains, above and beyond more traditional measures of mathematical and verbal ability.

"We live in the age of human capital," said Lubinski, professor of psychology. "Creativity is the currency of the modern era, especially in STEM disciplines. Having a better understanding of the human attributes that facilitate innovation has clear practical implications for education, training, business and talent development."

And yet, he says, despite longstanding speculation that spatial ability may play an important role in supporting creative thinking and innovation, there are very few systems in place to track skill in spatial reasoning.

"Current procedures for identifying intellectually precocious youth currently miss about half of the top 1 percent in spatial ability," Lubinski said.

Using data from a study that began in the late 1970s, Lubinski and colleagues followed up with 563 students who had scored exceptionally well -- in the top 0.5 percent -- on the SATs at age 13. The researchers also examined data on the participants' spatial ability at age 13, as measured by the Differential Aptitude Test.

Confirming previous research, the data revealed that participants' mathematical and verbal reasoning scores on the SAT at age 13 predicted their scholarly publications and patents 30 years later.

But spatial ability at 13 yielded additional predictive power, suggesting that early spatial ability contributes in a unique way to later creative and scholarly outcomes, especially in STEM domains.

These results confirm longstanding speculation in the psychological sciences that spatial ability offers something important to the understanding of creativity that traditional measures of cognitive abilities used in educational and occupational selection don't capture, the researchers concluded.

Lubinski believes cultivating these skills is imperative for ensuring scientific innovation.

"These students have exceptional and under-challenged potential, especially for engineering and technology," Lubinski explains. "We could do a much better job of identifying these students and affording them better opportunities for developing their talents."

Co-authors on this research include Camilla Benbow, Patricia and Rodes Hart Dean of Education and Human Development at Vanderbilt's Peabody College, James Steiger, professor of psychology and human development and Harrison Kell, postdoctoral fellow at Peabody College.
Share this story on Facebook, Twitter, and Google:

Other social bookmarking and sharing tools:
Share on stumbleupon Share on linkedin Share on pinterest_share Share on blogger Share on digg Share on delicious Share on newsvine | 24

Story Source:

The above story is reprinted from materials provided by Vanderbilt University. The original article was written by Jennifer Wetzel.

Note: Materials may be edited for content and length. For further information, please contact the source cited above.

Journal Reference:

1. H. J. Kell, D. Lubinski, C. P. Benbow, J. H. Steiger. Creativity and Technical Innovation: Spatial Ability's Unique Role. Psychological Science, 2013; DOI:

No comments: